home *** CD-ROM | disk | FTP | other *** search
- Path: bloom-beacon.mit.edu!hookup!swrinde!emory!nntp.msstate.edu!saimiri.primate.wisc.edu!news.doit.wisc.edu!post.its.mcw.edu!admin-one.radbio.mcw.edu!user
- From: jmoulder@its.mcw.edu (John Moulder)
- Newsgroups: sci.med.physics,sci.answers,news.answers
- Subject: Powerlines and Cancer FAQs (2 of 4)
- Supersedes: <jmoulder-281293174841@admin-one.radbio.mcw.edu>
- Followup-To: sci.med.physics
- Date: 25 Mar 1994 18:03:03 GMT
- Organization: Medical College of Wisconsin
- Lines: 506
- Approved: new-answers-request@MIT.edu
- Distribution: world
- Expires: 30 April 1994 00:00:00 GMT
- Message-ID: <jmoulder-250394120211@admin-one.radbio.mcw.edu>
- References: <jmoulder-250394115747@admin-one.radbio.mcw.edu>
- NNTP-Posting-Host: admin-one.radbio.mcw.edu
- Summary: Q&As on the connection between powerlines, electrical
- occupations and cancer (continued)
- Keywords: powerlines, magnetic fields, cancer, EMF, non-ionizing
- radiation, FAQ
- Xref: bloom-beacon.mit.edu sci.med.physics:1293 sci.answers:1012 news.answers:16848
-
- Archive-name: powerlines-cancer-FAQ/part2
- Last-modified: 1994/3/25
- Version: 2.4
-
- FAQs on Power-Frequency Fields and Cancer (part 2 of 4)
-
- 16) What do laboratory studies tell us about power-frequency fields and
- cancer?
-
- Carcinogens, agents that cause cancer, are generally of two types:
- genotoxins and promoters. Genotoxic agents (often called initiators)
- directly damage the genetic material of cells. Genotoxins usually effect
- all types of cells, and may cause many different types of cancer.
- Genotoxins generally do not have thresholds for their effect; in other
- words, as the dose of the genotoxin is lowered the risk gets smaller, but
- it never goes away. A promoter (often called an epigenetic agent) is
- something that increases the cancer risk in animals already exposed to a
- genotoxic carcinogen. Promoters usually effect only certain types of
- cells, and may cause only certain types of cancer. Promoters generally
- have thresholds for their effect; in other words, as the dose of the
- promoter is lowered a level is reached in which there is no risk.
-
- 16A) Are power-frequency fields genotoxic?
-
- There are many approaches to measuring genotoxicity. Whole-organism
- exposure studies can be used to see whether exposure causes cancer or
- causes mutations. Cellular studies can be done to detect DNA or
- chromosomal damage.
-
- Very few whole-organism exposure studies have been done. Bellossi et al
- [G13] exposed leukemia-prone mice for 5 generations and found no effect on
- leukemia rates; however, since the study used 12 and 460 Hz pulsed fields
- at 60 G (6 mT), the relevance of this to power-frequency fields is unclear.
- Otaka et al [G18] showed that power-frequency magnetic fields did not case
- mutations in fruit flies. Rannug et al [G19] found that power-frequency
- magnetic fields did not increase the incidence of skin tumors or leukemia
- in mice. RD Benz et conducted a multi-generation mouse exposure study in
- 1983-1985 as part of the NY State Powerlines Project; this study reported
- no increase in mutations rates or sister chromatid exchanges, but has never
- been published.
-
- A number of published laboratory studies have reported that power-frequency
- magnetic fields do not cause DNA strand breaks [G4,G16] chromosome
- aberrations [G1,G6,G15], sister chromatid exchanges [G2,G6,G11,G20],
- micronuclei formation [G9,G11] or mutations [G3,G15,G17].
-
- Many of the above laboratory studies also examined power-frequency
- electrical fields and combination of power-frequency electrical and
- magnetic fields [G1,G2,G4,G8,G11,G16]. As with the studies of magnetic
- fields alone, the studies of electrical fields and combined fields showed
- no evidence of genotoxicity.
-
- There are two positive reports of genotoxicity. Khalil & Qassem [G12]
- reported that a 10.5 G (1.05 mT) pulsed field caused chromosome
- aberrations. Nordenson et al [E4] reported that switchyard workers exposed
- to spark discharges had an increased rate of chromosomal defects, but
- Bauchinger et al [E2] for no such increase in chromosomal defects in a
- similar study.
-
- 16B) Are power-frequency magnetic fields cancer promoters?
-
- There are agents (for example, promoters) that influence the development of
- cancer without directly damaging the genetic material. It has been
- suggested that power-frequency EMFs could promote cancer [L1]. In a
- promotion test, animals are exposed to a known genotoxin at a dose that
- will cause cancer in some, but not all animals. Another set of animals are
- exposed to the genotoxin, plus another agent. If the agent plus the
- genotoxin results in more cancers that seen for the genotoxin alone, then
- that agent is a promoter.
-
- Published studies have shown that power-frequency magnetic fields do not
- promote chemically-induced skin cancer [G10,G14,G19] or chemically-induced
- liver cancers [G21,G24]. For chemically-induced breast cancer, one study
- has shown promotion [G22] and one has not [G23].
-
- 16C) Do power-frequency magnetic fields enhance the effects of other
- genotoxic agents?
-
- There are some other types of studies that are relevant to the carcinogenic
- potential of agents, but that are not strictly either genotoxicity or
- promotion tests. The most common of these are cellular studies that test
- whether an agent enhances the genotoxic activity of a known genotoxin;
- these studies are the cellular equivalent of a promotion study.
-
- Published studies have reported that power-frequency magnetic fields do not
- enhance the mutagenic effects of known genotoxins [G3,G9], and do not
- inhibit the repair of DNA damage induced by ionizing [G7,G8] or UV [G15]
- radiation.
-
- One study [G6] has reported that power-frequency fields can increase the
- frequency of sister chromatid exchanges induced by known genotoxins.
-
- 17) How do laboratory studies of the effects of power-frequency fields on
- cell growth, immune function, and melatonin relate to the question of
- cancer risk?
-
- There are other biological effects that might be related to cancer. There
- are substances (called mitogens) that cause non-growing normal cells to
- start growing. Some mitogens appear to be carcinogens. There have been
- numerous studies of the effects of power-frequency fields on cell growth
- (proliferation) and tumor growth (progression). Most recent studies of the
- effects of power-frequency magnetic fields on cancer progression have shown
- no effect [G5,G10,H3], but one has reported enhanced progression [G14].
- Most recent studies of effects of power-frequency magnetic fields on cell
- growth have also shown no effect [G1,G11,G16,G20,H2,H7,H8], but some have
- shown increased [G6] or decreased [G12] cell growth. With one possible
- exception [H1] there have been no reported effects on proliferation or
- progression for fields below 2000 mG (200 microT).
-
- Suppression of the immune system in animals and humans is associated with
- increased rates of certain types of cancer, particularly lymphomas [E6,E7].
- Immune suppression has not been associated with excess leukemia and brain
- cancer. Some studies have shown that power-frequency fields can have
- effects on cells of the immune system [K2], but no studies have shown the
- type or magnitude of immunosuppression that is associated with increased
- cancer risks.
-
- It has also been suggested that power-frequency EM fields might suppress
- the production of the hormone melatonin, and that melatonin has
- "cancer-preventive" activity [H6,H7,L2]. This is highly speculative.
- There have been some reports that EM fields effect melatonin production,
- but studies using power-frequency magnetic fields have not shown
- reproducible effects [H9,H10]. In addition, while there is some evidence
- that melatonin has "cancer-preventive" activity against transplanted breast
- tumors in rats, there is no evidence that melatonin effects other types of
- cancer, or that it has any effect on breast or other cancers in humans.
-
- 18) Do power-frequency fields show any effects at all in laboratory
- studies?
-
- While the laboratory evidence does not suggest a link between
- power-frequency magnetic fields and cancer, numerous studies have reported
- that these fields do have "bioeffects", particularly at high field strength
- [H4,H5,K1,K2]. Power-frequency fields intense enough to induce electrical
- currents in excess of those that occur naturally (above 5 G, 500 microT,
- see Question 8) have shown reproducible effects, including effects on
- humans [K1].
-
- Below about 2 G (200 microT) there are few published (and replicated)
- reports of bioeffects, although there are unreplicated reports of effects
- for fields as low as about 200 mG (20 microT). Even among the scientists
- who believe that there may be a connection between power-frequency fields
- and cancer, there is no consensus as to mechanisms which would connect
- these "bioeffects" with cancer causation [K1,L1].
-
- 19) What about the new "Swedish" study showing a link between power lines
- and cancer?
-
- There are new residential and occupational studies from Sweden
- [C12,C17,D7], Denmark [D9,C15], Finland [C14] and the Netherlands [C16].
- The published studies are considerably more cautious in their
- interpretations of the data than were the unpublished preliminary reports
- and the earlier press reports.
-
- The authors of the Scandinavian childhood cancer studies [C14,C15,C17] have
- produced a collaborative meta-analysis of their data [B6]. The RRs
- (Question 13) from this meta-analysis are shown below in comparison to
- meta-analysis of the prior studies [B4,B5].
- Childhood leukemia, Scandinavian: 2.1 (1.1-4.1)
- Childhood leukemia, prior studies: 1.3 (0.8-2.1)
- Childhood lymphoma, Scandinavian: 1.0 (0.3-3.7)
- Childhood lymphoma, prior studies: none
- Childhood CNS cancer, Scandinavian: 1.5 (0.7-3.2)
- Childhood CNS cancer, prior studies: 2.4 (1.7-3.5)
- All childhood cancer, Scandinavian: 1.3 (0.9-2.1)
- All childhood cancer, prior studies: 1.6 (1.3-1.9)
-
- - Fleychting & Ahlbom [C12,C17]. This is a case-control study of everyone
- who lived within 300 meters of high-voltage powerlines between '60 and '85.
- For children all types of tumors were analyzed; for adults only leukemia
- and brain tumors were studied. Exposure was assessed by spot measurements,
- calculated retrospective assessments, and distance from power lines. No
- increased overall cancer incidence was found in either children or adults,
- for any definition of exposure. An increased incidence of leukemia (but
- not other cancers) was found in children for calculated fields over 2 mG
- (0.2 microT) at the time of diagnosis, and for residence within 50 m (150
- ft) of the power line. The increased incidence of leukemia is found only
- in one-family homes; there is no increased incidence in apartments. The
- retrospective fields calculations do not take into account sources other
- the transmission lines. No significant elevation in cancer incidence was
- found for measured fields.
-
- - Verkasalo et al [C14]. This is a cohort study of cancer in children in
- Finland living within 500 m of high-voltage lines. Only calculated
- retrospective fields were used to define exposure. The calculated fields
- are based only on lines of 110 kV and above and do not take into account
- fields from other sources such as distribution lines, household wiring or
- appliances. Both average fields and cumulative fields (microT - years) were
- used as exposure metrics. The total incidence of childhood cancer was not
- significantly elevated for average exposure above 0.20 microT (2 mG), or
- for cumulative exposure above 0.50 microT-years (5 mG-years). A
- significant excess incidence of brain cancer was found in boys; the excess
- was due entirely to one exposed boy who developed three independent brain
- tumors. No significant increase in incidence was found for brain tumors in
- girls or for leukemia, lymphomas or other cancers in either sex.
-
- - Olsen and Nielson [C15]. This is a case-control study based on all
- childhood leukemia, brain tumors and lymphomas diagnosed in Denmark between
- '68 and '86. Exposure was assessed on the basis of calculated fields over
- the period from conception to diagnosis. No overall increase in cancer was
- found when 0.25 microT (2.5 mG) was used as the cut-point to define
- exposure (as specified in the study design). After the data were analyzed,
- it was found that the overall incidence of childhood cancer was
- significantly elevated if 0.40 microT (4 mG) was used as the cut-point. No
- significant increase was found for leukemia or brain cancer incidence for
- any cut-point. A significant increase in lymphoma was found for the 0.10
- microT cut-point but not for higher cut-points.
-
- - Guenel et al [D9]. This is a case-control study based on all cancer in
- actively employed Danes between '70 and '87 who were 20-64 years old in
- '70. Each occupation-industry combination was coded on the basis of
- supposed 50-Hz magnetic field exposure. No significant increases were seen
- for breast cancer, malignant lymphomas or brain tumors. Leukemia incidence
- was significantly elevated among men in the highest exposure category;
- women in similar exposure categories showed no increase in leukemia.
-
- -Floderus et al [D9]. This is a case-control study of leukemia and brain
- tumors in occupationally-exposed men who were 20-64 years of age in '80.
- Exposure calculations were based on the job held longest during the 10-year
- period prior to diagnosis. Many measurements were taken using a person
- whose job was most similar to that of the person in the study. About
- two-thirds of the subjects in the study could be assessed in this manner.
- A significant elevation in incidence was found for leukemia, but not for
- brain cancer.
-
- -Schreiber et al [C16]. This is a retrospective cohort study of people in
- an urban area in the Netherlands. People were considered exposed in they
- lived within 100 m of transmission equipment (150 kV lines plus a
- substation). Fields in the "exposed" group were 1-11 mG (0.1-1.1 microT),
- fields in the "unexposed" group were 0.2-1.5 mG (0.02-0.15 microT). The
- total cancer incidence in the ╥exposed╙ group was insignificantly less than
- that in the general Dutch population. No cases of leukemia or brain cancer
- were seen in the "exposed" group.
-
- 20) What criteria do scientists use to evaluate all the confusing and
- contradictory laboratory and epidemiological studies of power-frequency
- magnetic fields and cancer?
-
- There are certain widely accepted criteria that are weighed when assessing
- such groups of epidemiological and laboratory studies. These are often
- called the "Hill criteria" [E1]. Under the Hill criteria one examines the
- strength (Question 20A) and consistency (Question 20B) of the association
- between exposure and risk, the evidence for a dose-response relationship
- (Question 20C), the laboratory evidence (Question 20D), and the biological
- plausibility (Question 20E). These criteria are viewed as a whole; no
- individual criterion is either necessary or sufficient for concluding that
- there is a causal relationship between an exposure and a disease.
-
- Overall, application of the Hill criteria shows that the current evidence
- for a connection between power-frequency fields and cancer is quite weak,
- because of the weakness and inconsistencies in the epidemiological studies,
- combined with the lack of a dose-response relationship in the human
- studies, and the negative laboratory studies.
-
- 20A) Criterion One: How strong is the association between exposure to
- power-frequency fields and the risk of cancer?
-
- The first Hill criterion is the *strength of the association* between
- exposure and risk. That is, is there a clear risk associated with
- exposure? A strong association is one with a RR (Question 13) of 5 or
- more. Tobacco smoking, for example, shows a RR for lung cancer 10-30 times
- that of non-smokers.
-
- Most of the positive power-frequency studies have RRs of less than two.
- The leukemia studies as a group have RRs of 1.1-1.3, while the brain cancer
- studies as a group have RRs of about 1.3-1.5. This is only a weak
- association.
-
- 20B) Criterion Two: How consistent are the studies of associations between
- exposure to power-frequency fields and the risk of cancer?
-
- The second Hill criterion is the *consistency* of the studies. That is, do
- most studies show about the same risk for the same disease? Using the same
- smoking example, essentially all studies of smoking and cancer showed an
- increased risk for lung and head-and-neck cancers.
-
- Many power-frequency studies show statistically significant risks for some
- types of cancers and some types of exposures, but many do not. Even the
- positive studies are inconsistent with each other. For example, while a
- new Swedish study [C17] shows an increased incidence of childhood leukemia
- for one measure of exposure, it contradicts prior studies that showed an
- increase in brain cancer [B4,B5], and a parallel Danish study [D9] shows an
- increase in childhood lymphomas, but not in leukemia. Many of the studies
- are internally inconsistent. For example, where a new Swedish study [C17]
- shows an increase for childhood leukemia, it shows no overall increase in
- childhood cancer, implying that the rates of other types of cancer were
- decreased. In summary, few studies show the same positive result, so that
- the consistency is weak.
-
- 20C) Criterion Three: Is there a dose-response relationship between
- exposure to power-frequency fields and the risk of cancer?
-
- The third Hill criterion is the evidence for a *dose-response
- relationship*. That is, does risk increase when the exposure increases?
- Again, the more a person smokes, the higher the risk of lung cancer.
-
- No published power-frequency exposure study has shown a dose-response
- relationship between measured fields and cancer rates, or between distances
- from transmission lines and cancer rates. The lack of a relationship
- between exposure and increased cancer incidence is a major reason why most
- scientists are skeptical about the significance of the epidemiology.
-
- Not all relationships between dose and risk can be described by simple
- linear no-threshold dose-response curves where risk is strictly
- proportional to risk. There are known examples of dose-response
- relationships that have thresholds, that are non-linear, or that have
- plateaus. For example, the incidence of cancer induced by ionizing
- radiation in rodents rises with dose, but only up to a certain point;
- beyond that point the incidence plateaus or even drops. Without an
- understanding of the mechanisms connecting dose and effect it is impossible
- to predict the shape, let alone the magnitude of the dose-response
- relationship.
-
- 20D) Criterion Four: Is there laboratory evidence for an association
- between exposure to power-frequency fields and the risk of cancer?
-
- The fourth Hill criterion is whether there is *laboratory evidence*
- suggesting that there is a risk associated with such exposure?
- Epidemiological associations are greatly strengthened when there is
- laboratory evidence for a risk. When the US Surgeon General first stated
- that smoking caused lung cancer, the laboratory evidence was ambiguous. It
- was known that cigarette smoke and tobacco contained carcinogens, but no
- one had been able to make lab animals get cancer by smoking (mostly because
- it is hard to convince animals to smoke). Currently the laboratory
- evidence linking cancer and smoking is much stronger.
-
- Power-frequency fields show little evidence of the type effects on cells,
- tissues or animals that point towards their being a cause of cancer, or to
- their contributing to cancer (Question 16).
-
- 20E) Criterion Five: Are there plausible biological mechanisms that suggest
- an association between exposure to power-frequency fields and the risk of
- cancer?
-
- The fifth Hill criterion is whether there are *plausible biological
- mechanisms* that suggest that there should be a risk? When it is
- understood how something causes disease, it is much easier to interpret
- ambiguous epidemiology. For smoking, while the direct laboratory evidence
- connecting smoking and cancer was weak at the time of the Surgeon Generals
- report, the association was highly plausible because there were known
- cancer-causing agents in tobacco smoke.
-
- From what is known of power-frequency fields and their effects on
- biological systems there is no reason to even suspect that they pose a risk
- to people at the exposure levels associated with the generation and
- distribution of electricity.
-
- 21) If exposure to power-frequency magnetic fields does not explain the
- residential and occupations studies which show increased cancer incidence,
- what other factors could?
-
- There are basically four factors that can result in false associations in
- epidemiological studies: inadequate dose assessment (Question 21A),
- confounders (Question 21B), inappropriate controls (Question 21C), and
- publication bias (Question 21D).
-
- 21A) Could problems with dose assessment affect the validity of the
- epidemiological studies of power lines and cancer?
-
- If power-frequency fields are associated with cancer, we do not know what
- aspect of the field is involved. At a minimum, risk could be related to
- the peak field, the average field, or the rate of change of the field. If
- we do not know who is really exposed, and who is not, we will usually (but
- not always) underestimate the true risk [C13].
-
- 21B) Are there other cancer risk factors that could be causing a false
- association between exposure to power-frequency fields and cancer?
-
- Associations between things are not always evidence for causality. Power
- lines (or electrical occupations) might be associated with a cancer risk
- other than magnetic fields. If such an associated cancer risk were
- identified it would be called a "confounder" of the epidemiological studies
- of power lines and cancer. An essential part of epidemiological studies is
- to identify and eliminate possible confounders. Many possible confounders
- of the powerline studies have been suggested, including PCBs, herbicides,
- traffic density, and socioeconomic class.
-
- - PCBs: Many transformers contain polychlorinated biphenyls (PCBs) and it
- has been suggested that PCB contamination of the power-line corridors might
- be the cause of the excess cancer. This is unlikely. First, PCB leakage
- is rare. Second, PCB exposure has been linked to lymphomas, not leukemia
- or brain cancer.
-
- - Herbicides: It has been suggested that herbicides sprayed on the
- powerline corridors might be a cause of cancer. This is an unlikely
- explanation, since herbicide spraying would not effect distribution systems
- in urban areas (where 3 of 5 positive childhood cancer studies have been
- done).
-
- - Traffic density: Transmission lines frequently run along major roads, and
- the "high current configurations" associated with excess childhood leukemia
- in the US studies [C1,C6,C10] are associated with major roads. It has been
- suggested that power lines might be a surrogate for exposure to
- cancer-causing substances in traffic exhaust. This may be a real
- confounder, since traffic density has been shown to correlate with
- childhood leukemia incidence [E5]. Note that this would explain only the
- residential connection, not the occupational connection.
-
- - Socioeconomic class: Socioeconomic class may be an issue in both the
- residential and occupational studies, as socioeconomic class is clearly
- associated with cancer risk, and "exposed" and "unexposed" groups in many
- studies are of different socioeconomic classes [C13]. This is of
- particular concern in the US residential exposure studies that are based on
- "wirecoding", since the type of wirecodes that are correlated with
- childhood cancer are found predominantly in older, poorer neighborhoods,
- and/or in neighborhoods with a high proportion of rental housing [C18].
-
- 21C) Could the epidemiological studies of power lines and cancer be biased
- by the methods used to select control groups?
-
- An inherent problem with many epidemiological studies is the difficulty of
- obtaining a "control" group that is identical to the "exposed" group for
- all characteristics related to the disease except the exposure. This is
- very difficult to do for diseases such as leukemia and brain cancer where
- the risk factors are poorly known. An additional complication is that
- often people must consent to be included in the control arm of a study, and
- participation in studies is known to depend on factors (such as
- socioeconomic class, race and occupation) that are linked to differences in
- cancer rates. See Jones et al [C18] for an example of how selection bias
- could effect a powerline study.
-
- 21D) Could analysis of the epidemiological studies of power lines and
- cancer be skewed by publication bias?
-
- It is a known that positive studies in many fields are more likely to be
- published than negative studies (see Dickersin et al [E3] for examples from
- cancer clinical trials). This can severely bias meta-analysis studies such
- as those discussed in Questions 13 and 15. Such publication bias will
- increase apparent risks. This is a bigger potential problem for the
- occupational studies than the residential ones. It is also a clear problem
- for laboratory studies -- it is much easier to publish studies that report
- effects than studies that report no effects (such is human nature!).
-
- Several specific examples of publication bias are known in the studies of
- electrical occupations and cancer (see Doll et al [B5], page 94). In their
- review Coleman and Beral [B2] report the results of a Canadian study that
- found a RR of 2.4 for leukemia in electrical workers. The British NRPB
- review [B5] found that further followup of the Canadian workers showed a
- deficiency of leukemia (a RR of 0.6), but that this followup study has
- never been published. This is an anecdotal report, but publication bias,
- by its very nature, is usually anecdotal.
-
- 22) What is the strongest evidence for a connection between power-frequency
- fields and cancer?
-
- The best evidence for a connection between cancer and power-frequency
- fields is probably:
- a) The four epidemiological studies that show a correlation between
- childhood cancer and proximity to high-current wiring [C1,C6,C10,M2], plus
- the meta-analysis of the Scandinavian studies [B6].
- b) The epidemiological studies that show a significant correlation between
- work in electrical occupations and cancer, particularly leukemia and brain
- cancer [B1,B2,D7,D9].
- c) The lab studies that show that power-frequency fields do produce
- bioeffects. The most interesting of the lab studies are probably the ones
- showing increased transcription of oncogenes at fields of 1-5 G (100-500
- microT) [H4,H5,L1].
- d) The one laboratory study that provides evidence that power-frequency
- magnetic fields can promote chemically-induced breast cancer [G22].
-
- 23) What is the strongest evidence against a connection between
- power-frequency fields and cancer?
-
- The best evidence that there is not a connection between cancer and
- power-frequency fields is probably:
- a) Application of the Hill criteria (Question 20) to the entire body of
- epidemiological and laboratory studies.
- b) The fact that all studies of genotoxicity, and all but one study of
- promotion have been negative (Question 16).
- c) Adair╒s [F4] biophysical analysis that indicates that "any biological
- effects of weak (less than 40 mG, 4 microT) ELF fields on the cellular
- level must be found outside of the scope of conventional physics"
- d) Jackson╒s [E8] and Olsen╒s [C15] epidemiological analysis that shows
- that childhood and adult leukemia rates have been stable over a period of
- time when per capita power consumption has risen dramatically.
-
- 24) What studies are needed to resolve the cancer-EMF issue?
-
- In the epidemiological area, more of the same types of studies are unlikely
- to resolve anything. Studies showing a dose-response relationship between
- measured fields and cancer incidence rates would clearly affect thinking,
- as would studies identifying confounders in the residential and
- occupational studies.
-
- In the laboratory area, more genotoxicity and promotion studies may not be
- very useful. Exceptions might be in the area of cell transformation, and
- promotion of chemically-induced breast cancer. Long-term rodent exposure
- studies (the standard test for carcinogenicity) would have a major impact
- if they were positive, but if they were negative it would not change very
- many minds. Further studies of some of the known bioeffects would be
- useful, but only if they identified mechanisms or if they established the
- conditions under which the effects occur (e.g., thresholds, dose-response
- relationships, frequency-dependence, optimal wave-forms).
-
- 25) Is there any evidence that power-frequency fields could cause health
- effects other than cancer.
-
- While this FAQ sheet, and most public concern, has centered around cancer,
- there has also been suggestions that there might be a connection between
- non-ionizing EM exposure and birth defects. This concern has focused as
- much on video display terminals (VDTs) as on power lines. Little
- epidemiological or laboratory support for a connection between non-ionizing
- EM exposure and birth defects has been found. [J1,J2,J4,J5,J6]. Cox et al
- [J3] and Chernoff et al [K5] have recently reviewed this field.
-
- End: powerlines-cancer-FAQ/part2
-